Comparing the magnitude of two fractions with common components: which representations are used by 10- and 12-year-olds?

J Exp Child Psychol. 2010 Nov;107(3):244-59. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.008. Epub 2010 Jun 2.

Abstract

This study tested whether 10- and 12-year-olds who can correctly compare the magnitudes of fractions with common components access the magnitudes of the whole fractions rather than only compare the magnitudes of their components. Time for comparing two fractions was predicted by the numerical distance between the whole fractions, suggesting an access to their magnitude. In addition, we tested whether the relative magnitude of the denominator interferes with the processing of the fraction magnitude and, thus, needs to be inhibited. Response times were slower for fractions with common numerators than for fractions with common denominators, indicating an interference of the magnitude of the denominators with the selection of the larger fraction. A negative priming effect was shown for the comparison of natural numbers primed by fractions with common numerators, suggesting an inhibition of the selection of the larger denominator during the comparison of fractions. In conclusion, children who can correctly compare fractions with common components can access the magnitude of the whole fractions but remain sensitive to the interference of the relative magnitude of the denominators. This study highlights the fact that beyond the interference of natural number knowledge at the conceptual level (called the "whole number bias" by Ni & Zhou, 2005), children need to manage the interference of the magnitude of the denominators (Stroop-like effect).

Publication types

  • Comparative Study
  • Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't

MeSH terms

  • Analysis of Variance
  • Child
  • Concept Formation / physiology*
  • Female
  • Humans
  • Judgment / physiology
  • Mathematics*
  • Problem Solving / physiology*
  • Reaction Time / physiology
  • Surveys and Questionnaires